Discussion
Tom and Sherry Stevens
at time paper was
presented.
Abstract
A model of relationship happiness contrasts underlying beliefs of "taking care
of self" versus "taking care of others" as a primary affecting control dynamics
and happiness in relationships. If partners each assume primary responsibility for
their own emotions and happiness, then there is less need for coercion. Giving is
done for its own sake (versus obligation), communication is more positive and intimate
(since there is no need to coerce), decision-making is more equal, and each partner
has more freedom and independence to meet their own individual needs and interests.
When partners assume responsibility for each others' emotions, implications include
(1) each has the right to interfere in their partner's affairs and (2) each must
change their partner in order to be happy.
The Stevens Relationship Questionnaire (SRQ) was developed to test this model's
assumptions. We designed scales measuring Assertive Conflict Resolution (ACR), Intimate
Communication (Int), Romantic Attitudes and Behaviors (Rom), Equality (Eq), Independence
(Ind), and Liberated Role Beliefs (Lib). The SRQ was administered to 83 subjects
either (1) currently in sexually intimate relationships that had lasted for 1 year
or more or (2) separated or divorced from a relationship which met the first criteria.
Outcome/validity measures included Subjective Ratings--the Locke-Wallace
Marital Satisfaction Questionnaire and direct ratings of happiness--and Behavioral
Measures--sexual frequency, argument frequency, relationship longevity, and
relationship status.
The results were promising. Significant correlations with the Locke-Wallace included
ACR (r=.78), Int (r=.79), Rom (r=.53), Eq (r=.53) , and Ind (r=.24). Happiness ratings
followed a similar pattern. Sexual frequency significantly correlated with ACR (r=.44),
Int (r=.49), Rom (r=.44), and Eq (r=.26). Argument frequency significantly correlated
with ACR (r= -.39) and Int (r= -.28). Results of an ANOVA of relationship status
were also consistent with the model.
The model predicts that when people are understanding, genuinely caring, romantic,
and want equality and independence, they tend to be happier, have more sex, have
fewer arguments, and are more likely to stay together. The data generally support
this model of relationship satisfaction.
After a long day at work, a husband cooks dinner and washes dishes for his wife
and himself. How does he feel about it? Does he resent doing it, and do it only
because his wife has been "nagging" him to do it? Does he dislike doing it, and
do it because it is he thinks he "should" do it (and want to avoid feeling guilty
if he doesn't do it)? In either case, he will not find doing the dishes a very satisfying
activity, and may resent his wife a little more in the process (especially if she
does not always do her "duties.") As resentment increases, feelings of intimacy
(and intimate behaviors) are likely to decrease. As the husband feels and acts out
his resentment, it may increase the likelihood that his wife will respond with less
intimacy and more coercive behavior (to regain the lost intimacy). This cycle of
increased resentment and decreased intimacy may continue.
Return to Index
]
On
the other hand, what if he really felt good about cooking dinner and doing the dishes?
What if he chose to do the dishes for himself--not out of obligation to his wife?
What if he chose it because he focused on his love of his wife and appreciation
for her. What if he wanted to make it a gift to her and make fun to give her the
gift? What if he also focused on cooking because he was hungry? What if when he
chose to cook and do the dishes, he also accepted that it was his responsibility
to make cooking and doing dishes fun. He assumed that if he didn't enjoy cooking
and washing, that was his problem--not something to resent his wife for. If he cooked
and washed out of these motives that were oriented more toward making himself
happy--including feeling happy about giving a gift to his wife without expecting
anything in return--then he would not feel any resentment toward his wife. He might
even feel closer and more loving toward her as a result of this giving for its own
sake.
We believe that little daily events of resentment or loving such as these accumulate
to produce important long-term changes in how partners feel about each other. We
also believe that acting out of perceived coercion and/or attributed obligation
are major factors causing resentment.
If the husband perceives that he cooks and washes to meet his own needs or to
meet a genuine love for his wife, then he enjoys his tasks more, feels little (if
any) resentment, and is more likely to keep cooking and washing in the future--without
any coercion from his wife. Since he would present his gift without resentment
and with loving feelings, his wife would likely feel happy about receiving the gift
and respond with increased intimacy--not resentment and coercion as before.
This type of giving sounds great in theory. However, when we suggest that people
could have a marriage without doing anything out of obligation, most people's first
response is to imagine that this type of marriage would never work. They are so
used to doing things out of obligation, that they honestly believe that many of
the behaviors necessary to maintain a relationship would not be performed.
We are not suggesting that certain behaviors are not important to maintain relationships,
we are suggesting that the way people make their choices and think about
doing these behaviors is critical to motivation and marital happiness.
More generally we are suggesting that if each partner believes that he or she--not
the partner--is responsible for taking care of his or her own needs and happiness,
then this belief greatly reduces the likelihood of coercive behavior. If both partners
share this assumption and establish shared rules based upon this assumption, then
they can lay the foundation for both high independence and high intimacy. We like
to call this state "independent intimacy." An example of such a rule is that "Each
partner is responsible for his or her own actions, use of time, and emotional reactions."
"I will not assume that my partner is responsible for how I feel or for spending
time with me. We are together only when we both want to be together." This
is opposite the rule in many households that states, "We are together unless both
agree that being apart is ok." In the former situation a spouse can go out with
friends without permission. In the latter, permission must be granted. Each has
veto power. The result may be feelings by one or both partners--especially of people
with higher needs for independence--of being "trapped."
Return to Index
We have applied this principle to our own relationship for over 12 years, and
have applied it in clinical relationship counseling. Our experience has supported
this model of relationships. For example, a very common complaint of our clients
with relationship problems is that one of them is being too "needy," "dependent,"
or "smothering." Usually the person who is being too smothered is pulling away and
trying to increase distance and personal freedom. Helping the "needy" client understand
these dynamics and helping them develop a fundamental belief that he or she--not
the partner--is responsible for his or her happiness almost always improves the
relationship. Telling their partner of their new belief and acting on it almost
always has immediate positive effects. In our experience it has saved many relationships
that were about to end.
Our model of independent intimacy goes beyond basic beliefs in acting out of
enlightened self-interest and love versus acting out of obligation (independence).
It also includes the following factors: Beliefs, skills, and actions supporting
overall equality of decision-making; liberated roles that are relatively
gender-free; an assertive style (versus non-assertive or aggressive) for resolving
conflicts (that is minimally coercive and high on empathy and mutual concern);
intimate communication involving high self-disclosure of especially positive feelings
and very private events, but also of negative feelings and daily life events (empathy
is also important); and romantic themes supporting intimacy.
Return to Index
Let's briefly look at each of these factors.
Independence. Bentler and Newcombe (1978) discovered fewer problems with
independence or career conflicts with married couples than divorced ones in a longitudinal
study. Fitzpatrick and Indvik (1982) found that only couples that were classified
as independent (versus traditional or separate) had communication roles based upon
personal preferences as opposed to those expected by their partners. Gottman (1979)
found that nondistressed couples exhibited less reciprocity in their negative exchanges
than distressed couples. McLaughlin, M; Cormier, L.. S.; Cormier, W. H. (1988) found
that subjects in a high-marital-adjustment group had significantly lower levels
of distress, employed a greater number of coping strategies, and reported greater
frequency of use of coping strategies than subjects in the low-marital-adjustment
group. Overall, there appears to be little direct research relating independence
and autonomy to marital satisfaction.
Intimacy. Scheer and Snyder (1984) found that emotional distance significantly
correlated with relationship dissatisfaction. Jorgenson and Gaudy (1980), in a study
of 120 married couples, found that both disclosure perceived and given were significantly
(positively) related to marital satisfaction. Sprecher (1987) found that self-disclosure
of affection given and perceived predicted (positively) couples remaining together
over a four year period. Other studies have found similar results.
Romantic Attitudes and Behaviors. There have been a number of studies
linking variables such as attractiveness, thoughts about ones' partner, and mutual
affection with marital satisfaction and longevity. However, we could not find
one that was highly similar to our own scale (see below).
Assertive Conflict Resolution and Communication. A number of studies have
linked general variables of relationship communication with relationship satisfaction
and longevity. Scheer and Snyder (1984) found that communication problem-solving
skills and empathy positively related to marital satisfaction. Jacobson (1977) found
that teaching couples problem-solving communication skills increased marital satisfaction
(as measured by the Locke-Wallace Adjustment Scale). Markman (1981) found that a
measure of premarital couples' communication skills correlated .59 with relationship
satisfaction 5 1/2 years later.
Jacobson (1984) compared three behavioral marital treatments of distressed couples
with a wait-list control. Graduate student therapists gave 12-16 sessions. While
all treatment groups improved at post-test (wait-list did not), only the groups
receiving communication training continued to improve as measured by the six-month
follow-up. The other groups deteriorated. Billings (1979) found that distressed
couples engaged in more reciprocal negative communication than non-distressed couples
during conflict-resolution tasks.
Equality of Decision-Making. Bentler and Newcomb (1978) and Cattel and
Nestleroade (1967) were among a number of researchers who found that persons more
similar on scales measuring equality were more happily married. Grey-Little and
Burkes (1983) reviewed the literature and found that couples that were more egalitarian
tended to have higher relationship satisfaction.
Liberated Beliefs of Sexual Roles. Frank, Anderson, and Rubinstein (1979)
found that discrepancy between individual ideal and actual behavior (role strain)
was correlated with higher relationship dissatisfaction. Scheer and Snyder (1984)
found that liberated role beliefs correlated with a belief that the spouse is willing
to compromise. White (1981) found that jealousy was positively related to traditional
sex-role beliefs. These studies illustrate a possible relationship between liberated
role beliefs and marital satisfaction, but much more research is needed.
Return to Index
SRQ Questionnaire scales
The items for the SRQ were created from the following sources: current and previous
literature reviews related to each individual scale; personal experience in our
own relationship (an exceptionally happy one); clinical experience in individual
and relationship counseling with persons experiencing relationship problems; and
workshop materials from a series of workshops on communication, conflict-resolution,
assertiveness training, and the Robert Carkhuff-based HRD model. The SRQ may be
found in Appendix A.
The SRQ was designed to be face valid. No attempt was made to prevent faking
or compensate for a social desirability effect. This test was designed for persons
who are motivated to take the test honestly--it is of little value clinically for
others.
Independence=I. This is a 20-item scale measuring
freedom of both partners to make decisions and pursue individual interests and personal
growth.
Intimacy=N. This is a 12-item scale concerning
perceived communication of feelings, self-disclosure, likes and dislikes, and ideas
concerning respect, commitment, and common goals.
Romantic Attitudes and Behaviors=R. This
is a 7-item scale including items such as attraction, fantasizing, and special favors.
Assertive Conflict Resolution and Communication =A.
This is a 23-item scale measuring the use of positive, supportive, direct,
and empathetic statements during discussions related to various levels of problem
resolution. Assertive as opposed to non-assertive or aggressive responses are scored
higher.
Equality of Decision-Making=E. This is a
4-item scale measuring how equal the two partners perceive their decision-making
to be.
Liberated Beliefs of Sexual Roles=L. This
is a 6-item scale measuring non-traditional "liberated", egalitarian beliefs regarding
male and female roles within the context of an intimate relationship. It does not
include equality of decision-making or communication styles. It focuses upon lack
of role specialization.
Return to Index
Relationship Endurance. This is defined as the length of time of an intimate
sexual relationship.
Relationship Status. This is defined as an intimate sexual relationship
in which both partners are either currently involved with the target partner or
not.
Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Test. The Locke-Wallace is a 15-item,
self-report index of marital adjustment. It is a multiple-choice inventory derived
from the fundamental items of six marital adjustment tests used prior to 1959. It
has been widely used as a measure of marital satisfaction in research on relationships.
Bentler (1978), Edinger and Patterson (1983), Jacobson (1977, 1978), Markman (1979),
and Margolin and Weiss (1978) are a few of the researchers using Locke-Wallace in
their journal articles. The possible scores range from 2 to 158. Of the well-adjusted
group tested in the original validation study, 96% achieved scores of 100 or more.
The introductory statement of the Marital Relationship Inventory was revised
so that it would read appropriately for unmarried couples The use of the word "partner"
in place of "spouse" and "couples" instead of marital relationship.
Return to Index
Results
The SRQ Scales and Relationship Satisfaction and Behaviors
Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients were computed from the responses
of 83 volunteer male and female college students who took the Stevens Relationship
Questionnaire (SRQ). The purpose of the study was to determine if the following
variables were related to relationship satisfaction and endurance: independence,
intimacy, romantic attitudes and behavior, equality of decision making. Relationship
satisfaction global measures were questions 71 and 72 of the SRQ and were stated
as follows:
71. Overall, I have an extremely happy relationship with my partner.
72. Overall, I am extremely satisfied with my relationship with my partner,
Choices to each question ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
Scores on the six scales were correlated with behavioral measures of relationship
adjustment, measured by questions 73 and 74 on the SRQ:
73. My partner and I have sexual intercourse:
74. My partner and
I have arguments for at least several minutes.
Choices to both questions were as follows:
a. At least 5 times or more per
week.
b. At least 3-4 times per week.
c. At least 1-2 times per week.
d. At least 1-3 times per month.
e. Less than once per month.
The scores on the scales were also correlated to scores obtained from the Locke-Wallace
Marital Test. See Table I for Intercorrelations between the SRQ scales and Relation
Satisfaction criteria.
Independence Scale
Scores on the Independence scale correlated significantly with the Locke-Wallace
(r[83] = .24, p < .05), (See Table 1.) The Independence scale was correlated
to the global satisfaction rating items from the SRQ, questions numbers 71 and 72.
For item 71, "overall happiness" (r[83] = .18, p < .05), for item 72, "overall
satisfaction" (r[83] = .19, p < .05). (See Table 1.) Frequency of sexual relations
(item 73) did not correlate significantly (r[82] = . 03) with the Independence scale,
nor did frequency of arguments (item 74) (r[75] = .00, n.s.). (See Table 2.)
Scores on the Independence scale did correlate significantly with relationship
endurance (item 107), but in the opposite direction than expected (r[83] =-.21,
p < .05). Overall, the Independence scale presented a mixed picture. It correlated
significantly with the satisfaction criteria, not significantly with behavioral
items, and negatively with the endurance item.
Return to
Index
Intimacy Scale
Scores on the Intimacy scale correlated significantly with scores on the Locke-Wallace
(r[83] =.79, p < .OO1), "overall happiness" (r[831 = .65, p <.001), and "overall
satisfaction" (r[83] = .66, p < .001). (See Table 1.)
Frequency of sexual relations correlated significantly with scores on the Intimacy
scale (r[82] = .489, p < .001). Number of arguments (item 74) showed a significant
negative correlation (r[75] = -.28, p < .01) with scores on the Intimacy scale.
(See Table 2.) Scores on the Intimacy scale did correlate significantly with relationship
endurance (item 107), but in the opposite direction than expected (r[83] = -.22,
p < .05).
Overall, the Intimacy scale had moderately high correlations with the criteria,
and presented clear support for the research hypotheses, with the exception of relationship
endurance criteria.
Romantic Attitudes and Behaviors Scale
Scores on the Romantic Attitudes and Behaviors scale correlated significantly
with scores on the Locke-Wallace (r[83] =.53, p < .001). Scores on the Romantic
Attitude and Behavior scale were significantly correlated with "overall happiness
" (r[83] = .47, p < .001) and "overall satisfaction" (r[83] =.43, p < .001).
(See Table 1.) Frequency of sexual relations correlated .44 (p < .001) with scores
on Romantic Attitudes and Behaviors, whereas frequency of arguments showed no significant
relationship with scores on this scale. Scores on the Romantic Attitudes and behaviors
scale did correlate significantly with relationship endurance (item 107), but in
the opposite direction than expected (r[831 = -.46, p < .001). (See Table 2.)
Overall, the scores on the Romantic Attitudes and Behaviors scale had moderately
high correlations with the criteria, and were supportive of the research hypotheses
with the exception of relationship endurance.
Return to
Index
Assertive Conflict Resolution Scale
Scores on the Assertive Conflict Resolution scale did correlate significantly
with scores on the Locke-Wallace (r[83' = .78, p , .001). Scores on the Assertive
Conflict Resolution scale were significantly correlated with "overall happiness"
(r[83] = .61, p < .OO1) and with "overall satisfaction" (r[83] = .61, p <
.001). (See Table 1. ) Frequency of sexual relations correlated significantly (r[82]
= .44, p < .001) with scores on the Assertive Conflict Resolution scale, as did
frequency of arguments (r[75] = -.39, p < .001). Scores on the Assertive Conflict
Resolution scale did correlate significantly with relationship endurance (item 107),
but in the opposite direction than expected ( r[83] = -. 23, p < .05)- (See Table
2.)
Overall, the Assertive Conflict Resolution scale had moderately high correlations
with the criteria, and was highly supportive of the research hypotheses, with the
exception of relationship endurance.
Liberated Role Beliefs Scale
Scores on the Liberated Role Beliefs scale did not correlate significantly with
the scores on the Locke-Wallace (r[83] = .17, p > .05). Scores on the Liberated
Role Beliefs scale was significantly correlated to "overall happiness" (r[83] =
.22, p < ,05) and "overall satisfaction" (r[83] = .22, p < .05). (See Table
1.) Neither frequency of sexual relations nor frequency of arguments correlated
significantly with scores on the Liberated Role Beliefs scale. (See Table 2.) Scores
on the scale correlated significantly with relationship endurance, but in the opposite
direction than expected (r[83] = -.20, p < .05).
Overall, The Liberated Role Beliefs scale had low correlations with the criterion
items, but did support some of the research hypotheses. It did correlate with some
satisfaction criteria, but not behavioral items, and correlated negatively with
the endurance item.
Return to Index
Equality of Decision Making Scale
Scores on the Equality of Decision Making scale did (r[83] =.53, p < .001).
Scores on the Equality of Decision Making scale were significantly correlated with
"overall happiness" (r[83] = . 44, p < .001 ) and with "overall satisfaction"
(r[83] = .43, p < .001) , (See Table 1.) Frequency of sexual relations was correlated
significantly with scores on the Equality of Decision Making scale (r[82] = .26,
p < .01). No significant relationship was found between scores on the Equality
of Decision Making scale and frequency of arguments. Scores on the Equality of Decision
Making scale did correlate significantly with relationship endurance (item 107)
, but in the opposite direction than expected (r[83] = -.19, p < .05). (See Table
2.)
Overall, the Equality of Decision Making scale had significant correlations with
the criteria, except for the endurance item.
Return to Index
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between the six
scales on the Stevens-Snyder Relationship Questionnaire (SRQ) and measures of relationship
satisfaction and endurance. The study consisted of 83 volunteers attending California
State University, Long Beach, who had been in a relationship for a minimum of one
year. Each subject was given the SRQ and the Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Test.
The Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Test correlated significantly (in a positive
direction) with all of the scales except Liberated Role Beliefs. Self-reported frequencies
of sex and (fewer) arguments significantly related to scores on four of the SRQ
scales. Scores on the Independence and Liberated Role did not meet predicted relationships
to the behavioral frequencies. All of the scales significantly correlated to global
measures of relationships satisfaction and happiness.
The one type of measure which did not relate as predicted to the SRQ was relationship
endurance. All of the scales correlated negatively with endurance. However,
there is a possible explanation for this negative result. Many of the couples in
shorter relationships in the study were previously married or in previous long-term
relationships. It could be that the newer relationships in general were ones with
better communication patterns and other desirable characteristics. For example,
scores on the Locke-Wallace also had a correlation of -.13 with relationship endurance
(although it was not significant). The global happiness items were also not related
to endurance.
The ANOVA performed on the relationship status of the subjects sheds further
light on this issue. Relationships were classified as Married, Involved, or Separated/divorced.
There were significant main effects on all SRQ variables except independence (which
might be artificially high for separated/divorced people anyway). The interesting
parallel to the endurance measure is that the means for Involved subjects
were higher than means for Married subjects on several SRQ scales. Consistent
with predictions, the means on both Involved and Married groups tended
to be higher than for Separated/divorced subjects.
To measure reliability within the scales of the SRQ, the Cronbach alpha was calculated.
Four scales with moderately high reliability were Intimacy (.81), Romantic Attitudes
and Behavior (.71), Assertive Conflict Resolution (.86), and the Liberated Role
Beliefs (.79). Equality of Decision-Making, which had only four items, and Independence
had low reliabilities (.06 and .26 respectively). It appears that adding items to
the former and eliminating items in the 16-item Independence scale might improve
their alphas. In short, because of reliability problems there must be some doubt
about the results in which these scales were involved.
Overall the SRQ appears to be a promising instrument for marital satisfaction
criteria, both subjective measures of satisfaction and self-reported behavioral
frequency measures of sexual intercourse and frequency of arguments. The Intimacy
and Assertive Conflict Resolution scales were particularly promising. Their high
intercorrelations indicated either a high degree of overlap in what they measure,
or two highly correlated underlying relationship behavior sets.
One possible explanation of the high correlations between scores on the Locke-Wallace
and these two scales would be that the items were similar. However, in fact the
Locke-Wallace items were global relationship satisfaction items, while the two SRQ
scales tended to be much more specifically related to the behaviors of interest.
The Romantic Attitudes and Behaviors scale had a moderately high correlation
with attraction and sexual behavior, but also was promising in other areas. Even
the short 4-item Equality of Decision-Making scale had a moderately high correlation
with attraction and sexual behavior.
Liberated Role Beliefs seemed interesting, but was not as good a predictor of
relationship satisfaction. We purposely separated issues of equality of decision-making
and communication from the concept of liberated roles. That left only items related
to the types of roles, especially lack of gender-specific specialized roles. It
appears that decision-making equality and good communication are much better predictors
of relationship satisfaction than are the nature of the roles per se.
The Independence scale was the only one not to correlate with satisfaction, and
to have a low alpha reliability. One possible explanation of why the Independence
scale did not correlate higher with relationship satisfaction is the likelihood
that a subgroup of people in unhappy relationships became more independent in order
to escape being with their partner. On the Locke-Wallace there is an item that asks
if one would recommit to the same person, given the opportunity to start over again.
The Independence scale correlated ,30 with that item. (Correlations with other scales
ranged from .23 for Liberated Role Beliefs to .34 with Assertive Conflict Resolution
and .50 with Intimacy.) This may be some indication that people really do value
independence in a relationship.
One important issue that we did not investigate was the relationship between
the overall personal happiness of the individual in a relationship. That is a different
criteria than satisfaction with the other partner or the relationship as a whole.
Individual growth and happiness of each partner should be investigated in its own
right as a criteria separate from relationship criteria.
Further research on the validity of each individual scale to see if it measures
what it suggests by its description is also needed. For example, does the intimacy
scale really measure intimacy. That would probably require a series of studies.
Our own face validity approach came from our own personal and clinical experience
with hundreds of clients dealing with relationship issues. Whatever the Intimacy
and Assertive Conflict Resolution Scale measure, they do seem to measure something
about communication and seem to be good predictors of relationship happiness.
Our original notion is that taking care of one's own needs and happiness (as
opposed to expecting one's partner to take care of them) and giving out of empathy
and love (as opposed to obligation) sets the stage for improved empathy, caring,
personal freedom, and happiness (as opposed to obligation, guilty, and resentment).
We believe that these research results generally support the effectiveness of relationships
based upon independent intimacy.
Return to Index
NOTE: The original SRQ has been incorporated as
the relationship scales in the newer, much more comprehensive questionnaire, The
Success and Happiness Attributes Questionnaire (SHAQ). To complete all or
part of SHAQ, go to http:\\www.csulb.edu\~tstevens\success.
1. Brief version of paper presented at Western Psychological
Association March 31, 1995.