Science is an organized, social method for the production of reliable knowledge It is social in the sense that scientists build on the contributions of their predecessors and their peers The image of the mad, lone wolf scientist is way off! One trains a long time to absorb prior contributions and the culture of the scientific community Communication of results is critical to the scientific enterprise Progress reports at conferences to get feedback to improve the work before its publication Publication entails submitting your work to a panel of referees called on by the editor, who read your manuscript and recommend that the editor publish it, publish it after certain changes are made, ask that it be published in a journal better suited to it, or flat out reject it as crap Having survived the review process, your work once published is then open to the criticisms of your colleagues, including their attempts to replicate your findings It is also social in the sense that projects are often worked on by a team of collaborating scientists These are sometimes groups of peers Sometimes they are one or two principal investigators together with a team of technicians or students This is becoming more and more common as the equipment needed to do research gets more and more refined and expensive and as host institutions need to spread its usage among many scien- tists Not only is science a social affair, it is also a highly organized affair, and a lot of this lecture will focus on how science organizes its inquiries (more on that later) Scientists see themselves as producing knowledge, as much as discover- ing the truth You might wonder how one "produces" truth -- isn't truth just out there, unchanging, and waiting passively for us to "discov- er" it? Scientists have a particular view of "The Truth," which varies substantially from the common concept of it, and this leads to many misunderstandings between scientists and the larger society around them For scientists, truth is the highest and best interpretation of their observations, as agreed upon by the majority of qualified scientists working in a given field at a given time In other words, for science, truth is both consensual and change- able or provisional This drives some elements of the public nuts Most people conceive of truth as something eternal, unchang- ing, and, perhaps, revealed a long time ago through a religious figure Science accepts change in truth for the following reasons: Reality itself changes, forcing a change in our concepts Usually more importantly, our tools and technology change and this exposes us to information we couldn't get before and so forces us to change our views ... and allows us to ask different new questions Astronomy -- naked eye, Galileo's telescope, modern huge telescopes (e.g., Palomar), telescopes and scanners sensitive to other wavelengths than visible light radiation (e.g., radio, microwave, infrared, ultraviolet, X ray), satellites (e.g., Voyager and the outer solar system, its last image of the whole solar system, its participation in the supernova of 1987) Information processing technology -- computers, statis- tical methods (e.g., ALS LandBank) Geography -- field observation and hand-drawn maps, computerized cartography, computerized statistical methods and large-scale and multivariate data sets, geographical information systems, remote sensing, and computerized qualitative data reduction Our interpretations shift, too: more elegant explanations cover old and new information better Newtonian physics incorporated into and superseded by Einsteinian relativity Simple classification of life and the Lamarckian interpretation of evolution superseded by the "Modern Synthesis": Darwinian theory of evolution coupled with the Mendelian system of genetic inheritance and now the genome projects Crustal contraction and elaborate land bridge systems gave way to the plate tectonic theory, which ele- gantly explains the distribution and mechanisms of earthquakes and volcanoes, climate changes over the millions of years, and the distribution of closely related species across widely separated landmasses Scientists, then, accept that truth is changeable in these senses and that the majority of scientists will move toward higher and more sophisticated interpretations of real world observations When majority opinion among scientists in a given field shifts, then the "true" interpretation shifts Now one funny thing about this whole process is its humanness: Scientists like to think of themselves as logical individuals who can be moved to a higher and better truth through simple force of compelling logic, sort of Mr. Spocks These major shifts in scientific truth rarely take place in such a smooth and reasonable manner, however Scientists are people and, like people everywhere, they tend to be fond of their own opinions, particularly those to which they have devoted a lifetime of work The way research proceeds, as we'll see a bit later, is that everyone contentedly accepts the transmitted wisdom of the scientific culture and works out its minor details Through time, however, new observations pop up, which don't quite fit into the accepted world view They are often not even perceived: one sees what one expects to see These anomalies continue to pile up until they attract the attention of some, usually younger scientist One such individual will eventually come up with an interpretation of the anomalies, which integrates them with the older, more routine observations The old guard is aghast, because the new model quite possibly invalidates the premises of an older scientist's entire corpus of work: no human being easily accepts the revelation that s/he has been wrong all this time The old guard resists the new model, no matter how elegant it is, and only the young folks secretly pay attention and begin to work with it once they've safely gotten their Ph.D.'s under their belts As these young folks move into the powerful stages of their careers and as the old guard retires and dies off, the majority of scientists then shifts to the newer and higher truth through simple demo- graphics One last aspect of the definition of science given above: science is concerned with the production of reliable information Reliability depends on replicability Knowledge is reliable to the extent that one can repeat a given process and keep getting the same results: You have to be able to get a phenomenon to repeat itself consistently There are some phenomena that can't be observed directly and, hence, can't be repeated: for these, you have to get consistent repetitions of enough small scale aspects of the phenomenon -- testing in bundles, then With this understanding of science, consider the following issues: Is creationism scientific? Consensual truth among practicing life scientists Replicability Why do scientists avoid the UFO and psychic issues? Replicability Their existence is unpalatable, because they seem to violate a number of scientific truths that scientists as yet see no reason for discarding (e.g., relativity) Purpose of science To explain the world as far as possible without resort to the super- natural Science is, then, philosophically ... or at least operationally materialist (some scientists are philosophically religious but function operationally in a materialist manner in the scientific areas in which they do work) It holds that there is a "real world" out there, that that world is reflected in our senses and extensions of our senses, so that we can acquire knowledge about the "out there" It seeks to explain the material world by reference to material processes, those involving the interactions of matter and energy Another approach to the world is that of the various idealisms Idealism holds that there may or may not be a real world out there (some variants say yes, others no) Some philosophical idealists feel that what we perceive as the world is actually a reflection of our individual or collective minds and expectations (e.g., a lot of New Age thinkers, old time solipsists) Others hold that there is a real world, but that it was created by spirit or consciousness (religion would fall in here, the world being created by a deity) Still others say that the world out there is real, whether or not it was created supernaturally, but that the realm of human thought dominates material processes (this shows up in cultural geography, cultural anthropology, and sociology sometimes, in studies of how thoughts or beliefs create, say, economic change in and of themselves) Science, then, being philosophically or operationally materialist often finds itself inadvertantly upsetting some religious people, because religion very much depends on an idealist world view and so science seems to be threatening to some people (despite the philsopher Immanuel Kant's best efforts at keeping science and spirituality in their own corners, each dominating that aspect of human life at which they seem best suited, this back in 1781 in Critique of Pure Reason) The process of science The operation of science is historically contingent: it depends on the maturity of a given field of inquiry In youthful fields There is a rather unstructured search for anything that strikes you as "interesting" At this point, there is no theory to guide your opinion of what might be "interesting," so you let your heart be your guide, so to speak Such a field is highly "empirical," that is, focussed on facts, not theory It could also be characterized as "idiographic," that is, concerned with descriptions more than underlying laws Such a field is often focussed on categorizations, developing classification systems, as was, for example, biology in the late eighteenth century and much of the nineteenth ... or Landsat interpretation back in the mid 1970s Just because the field has no theoretical background at this point does not mean that one's work is unstructured in the sense of sloppy, however One is very painstaking and careful in collecting informa- tion and in communicating how one went about one's work This promotes replication and, hopefully, your work can be repeated and thereby supported As time goes on, patterns begin to emerge in the observations Classification systems seem better able to cover the things they're intended to Some of the regular patterns you see in the date can be stated as law-like statements: if you do this, you will consistently observe that At this point, we see the beginnings of theoretical work: The statement of elementary patterns These could be considered elementary generalizations The movement from data to generalization is called "induc- tion" An example of a field in this stage was AIDS research in the early 1980's, when no explanation for the disease had yet been identified, but there were consistent regularities in terms of related symptoms, associated micro-organisms, human groups most likely to experience the complex of symptoms, etc. As these generalizations are stated, they are tested back against "grubby reality" Some folks test the generalizations against virtually identical data sets simply to see if a particular study is replicable Others will say that if the generalization is valid, then a com- pletely different set of data could be expected to behave in thus and such a manner as a means of testing the original generalization: that is, they formulate and test a hypothesis If your results square up with expectation, fine and good If they don't, there are three possible reasons why: The generalization may be wrong You made an illogical connection between the original generalization and the new data it was tested against Either you just plain screwed up Or the connection revealed as illogical might itself turn out to lead in interesting directions Most common of all, you may have simply made an error in your data collection and analysis This movement from generalization back to the world of data is called "deduction," and takes the form of potentially allow- ing the generalization to be falsified Deduction usually proceeds by testing "if-then" statements. It allows us to test a generalization by seeing just how general it is: can it cover new data? A field that has begun to create and test law-like statements is said to be more "nomothetic" (law-seeking) than "idiograph- ic" (descriptive) As time goes on, this process of deduction is going to prune the number of generalizations, by validating some and disproving others The falsified generalizations are thrown out The survivors often show a pattern that itself can be stated as a higher level generalization So the field proceeds again by a higher level inductive episode, stating the laws that seem to link lower level laws or modelling the relations among them These higher level generalizations are also tested and weeded through deduction and themselves linked into even higher level generalizations, or theories A theory, then, is a high level generalization, one of great sophistication This is another point of conflict between scientific lang- uage and normal speech, leading to poor communication In popular parlance, a "theory" is "just" a "specula- tion" To a scientist, it is a sophisticated, high level grouping of regularities Popular speech has it that evolution is "just" a theory, so JQ Public thinks it's just a wild guess and so is creationism, so why not teach them both in science classes? High level theories are also subjected to testing in another deductive round This time the generalization is at such a high level that you can't test the whole thing at once in a single test So what happens at this point is that "hypotheses" are derived from the theory and tested singly or in bundles If the whole theory is right, then the following things should take place in a defined situation Each of the things is tested singly or all together in a related study project As time goes on, a given theory will withstand so many tests that virtually everyone in the field comes to accept it as the "truth" Such a theory is called a "paradigm" (pronounced "pair a dime") A paradigm is the theory which an entire field takes for granted as the accepted intellectual background for all the work going on in it You don't bother describing and defending the paradigm's basic logic in your articles any more You simply accept the theory and know that your audience is familiar with it, and try to extend it into new areas It is possible that, in the process of so doing, you may come up with truly anomalous results, which you then publish almost sheepishly You can't explain it within the paradigm, but in publishing it, you expect someone else will see a connection you didn't Over time, if consistent patterns appear in the anomalies and the anomalies are numerous enough, they may force a change in the paradigm or its overthrow when someone comes up with a better mousetrap Examples of paradigms: Physics: relativity and quantum mechanics Biology: the modern synthesis of evolutionary theory Geosciences: plate tectonics Note: there are no paradigms in the social sciences Partly because of their relative youth Partly because we are studying ourselves Partly because of the economic and cultural interests behind the possible results It is quite possible that because of these last two reasons, there will never be paradigms in the social sciences I should note that there are often debates in some fields about whether they should be idiographic or nomothetic at given stages of their development There is the possibility of premature generalization Such premature theorization is just wild speculation with little connection to the data Geography and other fields with a social science component had an embarrassing episode of this around the turn of the century up till about the 1920's Geographical determinism: physical environment deter- mines human social and genetic patterns Theory stated long before any data collected or lower order generalizations made This was inappropriate theorization based on the exten- sion of Darwinism to culture (a form of Social Darwinism) This neatly "justified" racism and colonialism It became obvious that it didn't fit the data once people bothered to go get the data The different definitions of the "perfect" envi- ronment, depending on which empire you were from Sequent occupance Geography was so burned by this that it retreated into pure empiricism and an idiographic method until the 1950's in the US Some archaeologists are making similar objections to theor- izing in their field Even as one can jump to the theoretical level too hastily, it is also possible to do the opposite: avoid theory and cling to mindless empiricism and thereby sterilize any inherent interest in your field The role of abstraction in science One of the biggest problems in science is the sheer complexity and confusion of the real world Abstraction one way around that: simplify the conditions under which you collect data or analyze it Methods of abstraction Laboratory experiments Computer simulations Thought experiments Principal components analysis and classification systems Generally involves analysis: Setting up your cartoon world by isolating it from grubby reality Stating your expectations in a rigorous manner Seeing if your expectations hold up under these special, isolated circumstances, if you in fact understand your system Once you've done this, you can plug grubby reality factors back into your cartoon world Synthesis If the model resembles the real world after a few rounds of syn- thesis, you may be justified in claiming you understand the basic factors underlying that reality Applications Science and technological development can eventually produce a body of tested theory and technological tools, which can then be applied to the resolution of human problems An applied and technological investigation, though it involves scientific concepts and theories, is actually quite distinct from science and is judged by different standards than pure science A technology-focussed investigation is justified, not by its situation in a body of theory, but in terms of the need it is designed to meet: So, if you were doing a planning study in a city planning office, perhaps a revision of the general plan safety element, you would review the laws that mandate general plans be made for California cities (social need) and the changes in our understanding of, say, seismic processes and seismic engineering (pure science and applied science) that force a revision of the safety element (social need again) If you were working on using fine-resolution hyperspectral imagery to identify evacuation routes, you'd refer to the social need of getting people safely out of the path of a fire and firefighters quickly in to the area where the fire is being fought (social need). You'd review the spatial, spectral, and temporal resolution of the imaging system you're working with and the software that allows you to process it (the science) and how you'd like to test out whether this is a feasible application (an open-ended endeavor more than a formal hypothetico-deductive exercise). |